The catarrhine who invented a perpetual motion machine, by dreaming at night and devouring its own dreams through the day.

  • 11 Posts
  • 2.38K Comments
Joined 11 months ago
cake
Cake day: January 12th, 2024

help-circle
  • They aren’t different sources of info, but parts of the same process. And they’re three:

    1. The utterance. Like you said, the words and how they’re arranged and such.
    2. Your internalised knowledge. It’s all that bundle of meanings that you associate with each word, plus your ability to parse how those words are arranged.
    3. The context. It’s what dictates how you’re going to use your internalised knowledge to interpret the utterance; for example, selecting one among many possible meanings.

    Without any of those three things, you get 0% of the info. They’re all essential.

    So no, it is not solipsistic at all, since it depends on things outside your head (the utterance and the context), and those are shared by multiple individuals.









  • I’ll copypaste an interesting comment here:

    [Stephen Smith] This article is a great example of a trend I don’t think companies realize they’ve started yet: They have killed the golden goose of user-generated content for short-term profit. // Who would willingly contribute to a modern-day YouTube, Reddit, StackOverflow, or Twitter knowing that they are just feeding the robots that will one day replace them?

    You don’t even need robots replacing humans, or people believing so. All you need is people feeling that you’re profiting at their expense.


    Also obligatory “If you’re not paying for the product, then you are the product”.


  • Lvxferre@mander.xyztoComic Strips@lemmy.worldLos Gatos
    link
    fedilink
    arrow-up
    9
    ·
    edit-2
    2 days ago

    Macanudo time? Macanudo time!

    1. Martincito has no friends.
    2. Until an imaginary person appears.
    3. They say that they’re called: Olga.
    4. And they have no friends either. [Speech bubble: “Hi Olga.”]
    5. Now Olga and Martincito are friendless together.

    1. Do you like this dress, Madariaga? (Madariaga is the teddy bear.)
    2. It’s my favourite.
    3. Which are your fav…
    4. :O
    5. Madariaga! You’re naked!
    6. Fellini! (Fellini is the cat.)
    7. Have you noticed that Madariaga is naked? He doesn’t wear cl…
    8. You too!
    9. And why would I cover this masterpiece?



  • We know that the modern /u:/ is not from that old /w/ because other words followed the same change - even words without /w/, like “moon”, “poop” (yup) or “boot”. In fact it’s how the digraph ⟨oo⟩ became associated with the sound.

    The case of “hwo”→“who” is a bit more complicated. As you said the “wh” digraph used to be “hw”; the change happened in Early Modern times, and it was likely for readability - less sequential short strokes = easier to read. People around those times did other weird stuff like respelling “u” as “o”, as in the word “luue”→“loue” (modern “love”), for the same reason.

    However, later on that /hw/ sequence of phonemes started merging into a single sound, [ʍ]: like [w] you round your lips to pronounce it, but like [h] you don’t vibrate your vocal folds. And if that [ʍ] happened before a rounded vowel - like [o:] or similar - it was reanalysed as a plain /h/. So for words like “who”, it’s like the “w” was dropped, just like in “two”, but in a really roundabout way.

    And, before non-rounded vowels, that [ʍ] still survives in plenty dialects; for example, “when” as either [ʍɛn] or [wɛn]. This change is recent enough that you still have some speakers in NZ and USA who use [ʍ].